Over-extension
Children
over-extend word meanings. This means that they will extend the meaning of one
category of item more broadly than it should be. An example of this might be
calling all round fruits ‘apples' when they are perhaps oranges, kiwis or
cherries.
Under-extension
Children
under-extend word meanings. This means that they will not accept that there are
more examples of a category of item than the particular one that is familiar to
them. So, for example, ‘dog' is used for the family pet but does not apply to
any other dog, thus narrowing the word's meaning.
Noun plurals
It is
generally accepted that children acquire language through an in-built ability
to recognise the patterns that exist. However, these patterns are not always
straightforward and there are exceptions, so children inevitably make mistakes.
Verb tenses
Young
children's speech will reflect some application of regular patterns, for
example, adding ‘ed' to form past tenses. However, as yet, irregularities will
not form part of their understanding, so birds ‘singed' and children ‘runned'
are completely understandable, if not completely accurate sentences.
Imitation & reinforcement theory
(Skinner, 1957)
Skinner was
a proponent of the theory that children acquire language by imitating the way
others speak. When the child is successful at producing words it is praised.
This approval motivates the child to repeat the action thus learning words.
This theory
has now been largely discredited. The task of acquiring language is such a vast
one – children acquire tens of thousands of words and complex rules of grammar
and syntax within a very short space of time.
Further
flaws in this theory are revealed if we consider the mistakes children make in
their grammar usage. They clearly do not imitate statements such as “I cleaning
my tooths”. This is not a sentence formation which would have been praised by
an adult.
Innateness theory (Chomsky, 1965)
Noam
Chomsky argued against Skinner's theory. He reasoned that children have an
innate ability to acquire language through what he called a ‘language
acquisition device' (LAD).
Chomsky
claimed that all languages have a different surface structure - French and
English sound different from each other through their differing intonations and
stresses. However, he felt that all languages share the same deep grammar
structure, or linguistic universals - subject–verb-object. His theory suggests
we are pre-programmed with this deep structure. Chomsky's theory explains how
children can understand sentences they've never heard before.
Critics
such as Bard and Sachs (1977) argue that children don't learn to speak
automatically. They need to communicate and interact with others – innateness
alone is not enough.
Cognition theory (Piaget, 1966)
The
cognition theory links stages in language acquisition with stages of cognitive
development. Piaget observes that children initially view themselves as the
centre of the universe believing that objects exist only in relation to
themselves. At around 18 months children begin to realise that objects have an
existence that is nothing to do with them. A big growth in vocabulary occurs at
this time and proponents of the cognition theory suggest that these events are
linked - children are compelled to find names for things they now know exist.
Piaget's theory shows a relationship between language and thought – though the
theory only seems to stand up for the first 18 months of a child's life.
Studies show that some children whose mental development is retarded can speak
fluently. Here it seems that word order, meaning and grammar have not been
subject to the child's general cognitive development.
Lenneberg 1967
What is the
cut off age?
As well as
theories on how children acquire language, there are theories about when. Eric
Lenneberg's (1967) theory suggests that there is a critical period in a child's
life during which they are able to acquire language. What is the duration of
this period? What evidence is there for and against it?
Lenneberg's
theory claims that language acquisition is linked to maturation. He proposes
that the human brain is designed to acquire language at a certain time.
Lenneberg
suggested that there is a cut-off age of around 12 or 13 years and that once
this period has passed language learning slowed down or in effect was no longer
possible.
The Science
The brain
begins to grow at around 18 months old. It has acquired grammar and phonology
by about four years of age and it is widely accepted that an ability to acquire
language probably extends this learning period up to the middle of the second
decade.
After this
point, it is said that there is a decline in the neural plasticity of this area
of the brain, severely impairing language acquisition ability.
Genie’s
story
The brain
begins to grow at around 18 months old. It has acquired grammar and phonology
by about four years of age and it is widely accepted that an ability to acquire
language probably extends this learning period up to the middle of the second
decade.
After this
point, it is said that there is a decline in the neural plasticity of this area
of the brain, severely impairing language acquisition ability.
Victor’s
story
Victor,
often referred to as a feral child was discovered at 12 years of age.
Initially, he was mute and again despite extensive treatment and a loving carer
he never learned to speak properly.
Criticisms
There are
criticisms of Lenneberg's theory:
Some ask
the question: Were Genie and Victor intellectually damaged from birth and
incapable of learning how to speak?
Others ask:
Were the speech therapists and specialists' methods inadequate in helping Genie
and Victor? Would they have fared better in the care of others?
Has his
theory been adapted?
It has been
widely accepted that there is some truth in Lenneberg's theory. The immense
language learning ability, which accompanies us through our early years, does
seem to be shut down by some kind of genetic programming around the time of
puberty.
Genie and
Victor's cases would have also supported a theory by another well-known
psychologist - Vygotsky.
Vygotsky
(1978), argued that a child is only able to acquire language when he is
interacting with people in his environment and, in particular, in co-operation
with his peers. Obviously, neither Genie nor Victor had the opportunity to do
this.
Katherine Nelson (1973)
Katherine identified four categories for the first words that a child speaks:
- Naming (60% of first words were nouns from this group)
- Actions (verbs formed the second largest group and were used with actions or location words like 'up' and 'down'
- Describing
- Personal (made up about 8% of the sample)
This shown that first words were often proper or concrete nouns
Summary
The first 3
stages
- Crying: a child's only form of communication in the early weeks of life.
- Cooing: through which a child gains control of their vocal cords.
- Babbling: where reduplicated monosyllables (mama dada) often sound like a child is calling a parent.
Further
stages
- One-word stage: first words usually reflect a child's environment and they are often holophrastic in meaning.
- Two-word stage: demonstrates a child's first sentences and contains a primitive grammar.
- Telegraphic stage: sees utterances made up of words that tell us the main message but leave small unimportant bits out.
No comments:
Post a Comment